Issue |
Parasite
Volume 30, 2023
|
|
---|---|---|
Article Number | 49 | |
Number of page(s) | 10 | |
DOI | https://doi.org/10.1051/parasite/2023049 | |
Published online | 12 November 2023 |
Research Article
Varroa destructor relies on physical cues to feed in artificial conditions
Varroa destructor s’appuie sur des signaux physiques pour se nourrir dans des conditions artificielles
1
Laboratoire Évolution et Diversité Biologique, UMR5174, CNRS-Université de Toulouse III-IRD, Université Paul Sabatier, 31077 Toulouse, France
2
M2i Biocontrol–Entreprise SAS, 46140 Parnac, France
3
Conservatoire National des Arts et Métiers (CNAM), Unité Métabiot, 22440 Ploufragan, France
* Corresponding author: angelique.vetillard@lecnam.net
Received:
20
January
2023
Accepted:
24
October
2023
Olfaction is a major sense in Varroa destructor. In natural conditions, it is known that this honey bee parasite relies on kairomones to detect its host or to reproduce. Yet, in artificial conditions, the parasite is able to feed and survive for a few days even though most honey bee pheromones are lacking. Other key cues are thus probably involved in V. destructor perception of its close environment. Here, we used several artificial feeding designs to explore the feeding behaviour of the parasite when it is deprived of olfactory cues. We found that V. destructor is still able to feed only guided by physical cues. The detection of the food source seems to be shape-related as a 3D membrane triggers arrestment and exploration more than a 2D membrane. The tactile sense of V. destructor could thus be essential to detect a feeding site, although further studies are needed to assess the importance of this sense combined with olfaction in natural conditions.
Résumé
L’olfaction est un sens prépondérant chez Varroa destructor. En conditions naturelles, ce parasite de l’abeille domestique dépend en effet de kairomones qui lui permettent de détecter son hôte ou de se reproduire. Pourtant, lorsqu’il se retrouve en conditions artificielles, le parasite se nourrit et survit plusieurs jours malgré l’absence de la majorité des phéromones émises par l’abeille. Des indices clés autres qu’olfactifs sont donc très probablement impliqués dans la perception de l’environnement de V. destructor. Dans cette étude, plusieurs dispositifs d’alimentation artificielle ont été testés afin d’explorer le comportement de nourrissage du parasite lorsqu’il est privé d’indices olfactifs. Les résultats montrent que V. destructor est tout à fait capable de se nourrir en étant uniquement guidé par des indices physiques. En l’occurrence, la détection de la source nutritive semble être liée à sa forme puisqu’une membrane 3D provoque des comportements exploratoires plus prononcés qu’une membrane plane (2D). Le sens du toucher serait donc essentiel à V. destructor pour trouver son site de nourrissage. Des études complémentaires permettraient néanmoins d’évaluer les importances relatives des sens olfactif et tactile en conditions naturelles.
Key words: Artificial conditions / Olfaction / Physical cues / Shape / Pulse / Varroa destructor
© V. Piou et al., published by EDP Sciences, 2023
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Introduction
The foraging and feeding behaviour of mites remains poorly studied although it is of great interest in plant, animal and human health [23]. Through the predation of pests [7] or through the parasitism of plant and animal hosts [14, 62], mites can have significant agroecological impacts [23, 66]. Many cues are used by mites to find and feed on their host/prey, among which CO2, vibrations and pheromones have been shown to be relevant [13, 22, 39, 41, 56, 70]. Since many mites from the Parasitiformes super-order are blind, olfaction is a crucial sense for them to perceive the environment [24, 25, 39]. The effect of olfactory cues has been studied in mites from the genus Phytoseiidae such as in Phytoseiulus persimilis, which play an important role as biocontrol agents [2, 32–34, 69]. In these predatory mites, the detection of kairomones emitted by both their phytophagous prey and the wounded plant was shown to impact mite orientation [9, 58]. Although physical cues tend to be overlooked, several studies have highlighted the role they can play on mites [18, 22]. More specifically, heat, CO2 and vibrations are known to trigger host finding behaviours in several tick or mite species [1, 17, 41, 65]. In Varroa destructor (Parasitiformes, Mesostigmata), as in many other species, both chemical and physical cues can be detected by the parasite, allowing it to synchronise its lifecycle to the ecology of its hosts [10, 35, 45].
Varroa destructor is the main ectoparasite of the western honey bee Apis mellifera. Adult females feed on larval, pupal and adult bees, and transmit viruses in the process [60, 63]. Along with pesticide use, it is one of the main factors associated with colony losses in the Northern hemisphere [37]. Its life cycle is well described in the literature, although many behavioural aspects are not yet fully understood [11, 52]. As in Phytoseiid mites, olfaction seems to be the most important sense used by V. destructor [16, 28, 35, 51]. Many semiochemicals directly emitted by the bee, or coming from the colony matrices can be attractive to the parasite. For instance, specific blends of fatty acid esters from old bee larvae [6, 28] or other kairomones from cocoons and brood food [12, 36, 45] were shown to trigger arrestment (i.e., an undirected change of locomotor activity as defined by Kennedy [27]) or the attraction of female V. destructor. The foreleg has been described as the major sensory organ in V. destructor, with the sensory pit concentrating chemo, thermo and hygroreceptors [10]. By blocking them mechanically, males are disoriented and females become unable to find a suitable host [10, 38].
Due to the significance of chemical factors in the parasite’s lifecycle, other physical cues have received less attention from researchers. Yet, temperature and humidity were shown to be detected by V. destructor [29]. The size of the brood cell was also highlighted as a criterion that could be important in the host choice behaviour [31]. Because of the difficulty to isolate V. destructor from its chemical environment, knowledge about the relevance of physical cues in their foraging and feeding behaviours is still lacking.
Laboratory rearing methods offer a useful tool to try disentangling the role of olfaction from the role of other senses in the honey bee parasite. Several methods to artificially maintain V. destructor females in laboratory conditions have been described, some of which do not require the presence of honey bee host [46, 57]. In these artificial conditions, the parasite must move, feed and survive with no or limited help from olfactory molecules naturally occurring within a bee colony. These systems offer a unique opportunity to investigate if V. destructor is able to locate a feeding site using senses other than olfaction.
In this study, we aimed to examine whether olfaction is required for the honey bee parasite to find a potential food source in artificial conditions. Firstly, we tested whether V. destructor females were able to feed on a synthetic membrane covering a liquid solution based solely on physical cues. More precisely, the importance of parameters such as chemical cues and humidity coming from the food source, the bee or a Parafilm™ membrane was investigated. Secondly, we tested if blocking the parasite major sensory organ impacted their ability to find the artificial food source. Finally, on a shorter timescale and in specifically designed arenas, we recorded and analysed the locomotor behaviour of the mite when facing such artificial food sources. The aim was to elucidate how the liquid feeding solution could be detected by V. destructor. The role of physical cues in parasite orientation is explored and discussed in this work.
Materials and methods
Our studies were conducted according to European ethics laws for scientific research currently in force (Directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and the Council of 22 September 2010 on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes).
Biological material
Varroa destructor adult females were collected in brood frames from eight A. mellifera colonies kept on the university campus (INU Champollion, Albi, France) from March to September 2021 and 2022. Briefly, sealed brood frames containing late pupal stages (light to dark pigmentation of the body) were taken to the laboratory. At this stage, the mites are morphologically and physiologically close to dispersal mites and suitable to study survival and locomotion, compared to early reproductive swollen mites. The cells were opened with tweezers and V. destructor females were collected. The foundresses were then kept in a Petri dish on lightly to darkly pigmented pupae for 2 h (34 °C, 20% RH) until the start of the experiment. The same sampling method was used for artificial feeding and behavioural bioassays.
Artificial feeding experiments
Feeding chambers
To make the artificial feeding chambers, the methodology described in Posada-Florez et al. (2020) [46] was followed, with gelatine capsules rather than plastic microtubes (Fig. 1). First, a 6 mm hole was shaped in 1.37 mL gelatine capsules heads (LGA, La Seyne-sur-Mer, France). Two breathing holes were pierced with a needle in the body of the capsules. Sterilisation of the artificial chamber for 20 min under a UV light was then performed. Under a filtering fume hood, a 4 × 4 cm piece of Parafilm™ (Bemis, Co., Inc., Oshkosh, WI, USA) was fully stretched to around 16 μm thick as in Posada-Florez et al. (2020) [46] and Ramsey et al. (2019) [50]. It was then applied directly onto the 6 mm hole to create a small Parafilm well (Fig. 1, step 2). Twenty microliters of an FCF blue dyed nutritive solution were deposited in this well. A second membrane of Parafilm was used to seal the well and create a bubble containing the nutritive solution (Fig. 1). Similarly to Ramsey et al. (2019), a pigmented bee pupa was rubbed on the outer surface of the bubble before introduction of V. destructor females in the artificial chamber. In our study, two parasites were introduced in the capsule and the sealed capsule was transferred to an incubator for 48 h (34 °C, 70% RH). After 24 h in the artificial chamber, the feeding status and survival of the parasite were checked. The feeding success was assessed by the detection of blue dye inside V. destructor gut either externally or through the dissection of the parasite [44]. The survival status was also noted at 48 h.
Figure 1 Experimental set up used in the artificial feeding experiments. The feeding chambers are made from a gelatine capsule and Parafilm™ membranes. FCF blue dye is included in the nutritive solution to allow for the observation of V. destructor feeding status. |
Parameters tested in the feeding chamber
In this set of experiments, the general methodology to make artificial chambers was followed (Fig. 1), but some parameters were modified to determine their importance in the detection of the feeding site. The control nutritive solution consisted of a mix of cell culture media, as described in Posada-Florez et al. (2020) [46] (30% Schneider’s medium, 30% CMRL-1066, 15% blue dyed sterile water, 10% foetal bovine serum, 10% TC-100 insect medium, 4% Insect medium supplement and 1% Hanks salt solution). In a second group, the solution remained the same but the importance of olfactory cues coming directly from the honey bee was addressed by suppressing the rubbing of a bee pupa on the outer surface of the Parafilm membrane (Table 1). As Parafilm can be porous to small volatile molecules coming from the nutritive solution, the complex nutritive medium was replaced by a PBS solution coloured with FCF Blue dye in a third group (5 μL of 1% Blue FCF solution (Vahiné, Avignon, France) in 1 mL of PBS). A fourth group in which the walls of the capsule were entirely covered with Parafilm was also tested. In this condition, the parasite was surrounded by Parafilm and could not rely on putative olfactory cues coming directly from it. The negative control consisted of V. destructor females in gelatine capsules containing only a piece of Parafilm, without any food or liquid. Although the Parafilm membrane is waterproof, the impact of an alternative source of humidity was also tested in these artificial conditions (Supplementary Figure S1). The survival and feeding success of the parasite were checked, as previously described.
Experimental groups from the artificial feeding experiment with parameters tested and sample sizes. The sample sizes are higher for three of the groups because they were used to assess the survival rates of the parasite.
Deprivation of V. destructor sensory pit
In this experiment, V. destructor sensing was impaired by covering the tips of the forelegs with nail polish, as in previous studies [20, 38]. Black nail polish (Bérangé, Paris, France) was applied on cold anaesthetised female mites using entomological pins. This experiment enables blocking of the sensory pit organ, involved in odour, temperature and humidity detection [10, 38]. To control the effect of nail polish, a second group of females with a nail polish mark on the dorsal scutum was also tested. Two mites from the same treatment group were introduced in each feeding chamber containing a blue dyed PBS bubble. The feeding status of the parasites was noted after 24 h in both groups (n = 44 in the test group and n = 37 in the control marked group).
Behavioural observations
To further investigate the ability of V. destructor to detect an artificial feeding site, the parasite’s locomotor response to a liquid covering membrane was analysed on a shorter timescale. These behavioural observations were complementary to the 24 h feeding experiment as they can be used to determine whether the finding of the feeding site was the result of a specific foraging behaviour.
Design of testing arenas
The 13 × 7 mm arenas were made from Plexiglas square pieces (0.5 × 4 × 4 cm). First, two 4 mm wide holes were pierced through the Plexiglas, 2 mm apart from one another (Fig. 2). A first hole was used to generate a bubble of Parafilm filled with blue dyed PBS and the second one to make a membrane-covered chamber filled with the same PBS solution (Fig. 2). The surface of Plexiglas square was also covered with a thin layer of Parafilm to create a homogenous surface with low electrostatic reactions, as they were previously shown to impact V. destructor [43]. The Parafilm was always stretched to reach a thickness of around 16 μm [46, 50].
Figure 2 Steps followed to make the experimental arena used in the behavioural bioassays. The 13 × 7 × 2 mm arena is covered with Parafilm. A bubble and a discoid membrane covered well are formed. Both contain the same Blue FCF dyed PBS solution. The tested female V. destructor is placed in the arena between the second and the third step, right before the start of the recording. |
Four Parafilm walls (2 mm high) were then added to define the 13 × 7 mm arena (Fig. 2). A microscope glass slide was finally placed on the top of the walls to seal the arena, while allowing the clear observation of V. destructor females from above. Together, these components formed the final set up that was placed under a stereomicroscope to record the parasite’s locomotor behaviour (Fig. 2). The arena was thus composed of three zones. The first two zones, the disc and the bubble were named after the shape of the Parafilm membrane covering the PBS solution. A third larger zone, referred to as rest of the arena, included the floor and the walls (Supplementary Figure S2).
Recording and behavioural analyses
The arena was placed under a stereomicroscope (Leica S8APO, Leica Microsystemes, France), which was connected to a camera (Leica MC 190HD) and a screen (Iiyama). A female adult V. destructor was inserted in the arena and its movements were recorded for 10 min. The videotracking of the parasite over 10 min was then analysed using EthoVision® XT 17 (Noldus Information Technology BV, Wageningen, Netherlands [40]). The proportion of time spent in each zone (bubble, disc or rest of the arena) along with the number of stops, the duration of stops and the velocity were all measured directly by the software. Based on preliminary observations, the females were considered immobile if they moved less than 0.2 mm between two track points. When the parasite was immobile, short but clear vibrations or pulsations of its body were frequently observed (Movie S1). These pulsating behaviours appeared to be similar to those described by Hall et al. 2022 [19]. We estimated their frequency by counting the number of times the behaviour was expressed in a zone and weighting it by the time spent in each zone.
Statistical analyses
The results were analysed using R.4.0.4 [49] and graphs were generated using the ggplot2 package [68].
Generalised linear models (GLMs) with Binomial distribution were used to analyse the binary data resulting from the artificial feeding experiments. The feeding success and the survival rate at 24 and at 48 h were compared according to the rearing condition of the V. destructor females. If significant differences were detected, the groups were further compared using the multcomp package in R. Some conditions resulted in 100% fed female mites, so the parasite feeding rate was analysed using a bias-reduced GLM.
The parameters considered in the videotracking experiments were measured per zone of the arena. Because in the case of random movements, the probability to be in a zone is proportional to the surface of the zone, our measurements of stops and durations by zone had to be weighed by the surface of each zone. The analyses thus focus on these ratios that should not differ if the movements and behaviours of the parasite in the arena are random. When the residuals allowed it – namely in the case of the number of stops and number of pulses – linear models were performed and post-hoc analyses were run to further explore the differences. Alternatively, for the proportion of time spent in each zone and the duration of stops, Kruskal–Wallis analyses were used and paired data Wilcoxon tests allowed us to further assess the pairwise differences between zones. The significance threshold was 0.05, except in pairwise analyses for which Bonferroni corrections had to be applied.
Results
Effect of the restriction of chemical cues on V. destructor feeding success
Feeding in control and modified chambers
The feeding success was assessed by the detection of Blue dye inside V. destructor gut either externally or through the dissection of the parasite. The feeding success rates remained high regardless of the treatment and no significant differences were found between them (bias-reduced GLM, df = 3, χ2 = 4.25, p = 0.24). In the control condition, V. destructor females were able to detect the feeding site and to ingest the solution through the membrane in more than 90% of the cases (92.9%, N = 140, 95% conf. interval: 87.3–96.5; Fig. 3). When no honey bee odour was present in the system, the parasite was still able to find the food source and feed on the artificial medium (100%, N = 32, 95% conf. interval: 89.1–100). The food solution or the Parafilm membrane could represent sources of olfactory cues as well. However, in our study, V. destructor females were still able to feed when surrounded by Parafilm (96.4%, N = 28, 95% conf. interval: 81.7–99.9), when the food source is replaced by a simple saline solution (93.5%, N = 94, 95% conf. interval: 86.3–97.6) or when an alternative source of humidity is present (Supplementary Data S1).
Figure 3 Feeding success of artificially fed V. destructor females. Percentage and 95% conf. interval of females that had fed 24 h after the start of the experiment, in relation to the experimental set up used (Control feeding (n = 140), Control feeding w/o rub (n = 32) PBS (n = 94) PBS + Parafilm (n = 28). Letters indicate the statistical significance between groups. |
Survival in control and modified chambers
Twenty-four hours after the start of the experiment, the survival rate of V. destructor females fed on the artificial nutritive solution (positive control) was high (86.4%, 95% conf. interval: 79.6–91.6) but decreased to 62.3% the following day (Fig. 4). Starved females with no access to any food source survived for 24 h in 53.7% of cases (95 conf. interval: 42.3–64.7), but the survival then dropped to 9.8% after 48 h (95% conf. interval 4.3–18.3). Parasites can survive for a limited amount of time by ingesting only a PBS solution. The survival rate of these females was indeed significantly higher than the negative control and lower than the positive control (Fig. 4). This effect was observed at 24 (72.3% 95% conf. interval: 62.2–81.1) and 48 h (44.6% 95% conf. interval: 33.0–56.6). The differences between the three groups were significant for both times considered (T24 h GLM df = 2, χ2 = 28.6, p < 0.001; T48 h GLM df = 2, χ2 = 65.1, p < 0.001).
Figure 4 Survival of V. destructor females in artificial conditions. Survival rates and 95% conf. interval of females (in %) 24 h and 48 h after the start of the experiment, in relation to the experimental set up used (Control feeding n = 140; PBS n = 94; Negative control n = 82). Letters indicate the statistical significance between groups. |
Feeding of sense-deprived females V. destructor
When the females’ forelegs were covered with nail polish, the feeding success rate was still high, with 86.4% (95% conf. interval: 72.6–94.8) of the parasites that fed after 24 h (Fig. 5). This was not significantly different from the control parasites marked with nail polish on their dorsal scutum (81.1%, 95% conf. interval: 64.8–92.0; GLM Df = 1, χ2 = 0.42, p = 0.52). The mites were still able to detect the feeding site when deprived of the sensory pit.
Figure 5 Effect of sensory pit organ deprivation on the ability of V. destructor to find a food source in artificial conditions. The graph shows the percentage and 95% conf. interval of varnished (n = 44) and control (n = 37) parasites that fed after 24 h in artificial conditions. Letters indicate the statistical significance between the two groups. |
Behavioural observations
The behavioural analyses revealed that the parasites spent a high amount of time on the bubble (mean ± SE: 55.2 ± 6.1% of the 10 min), whereas they mostly ignored the flat Parafilm membrane covering the PBS-filled well (4.9 ± 1.3%, Fig. 6). The amount of time spent on the bubble is actually due to an arresting effect of the hemispheric surface [27], which translates into a reduction or a stop of locomotion when in contact with the bubble surface. Females stopped more often (10.3 ± 1.9) and for longer periods (285.3 ± 40.9 s) when on the bubble compared to the flat membrane (1.9 ± 0.9 and 6.6 ± 3.2 s, Fig. 6). The effect of the spherical surface of the bubble is confirmed by the behaviour of parasites in the rest of the arena. Indeed, the proportion of time spent in the arena (39.9 ± 5.6%) was almost exclusively spent on walls of Parafilm that also happened to have a rounded surface. The number and duration of stops on walls were in between the values of the two other zones (8.1 ± 1.4 and 149.3 ± 30.7 s). To take into account the impact of the zone surface on our results, ratios of parameters weighed by the surface were calculated. The differences are even more striking and the ratios of the amount of time (Kruskal–Wallis df = 2, χ2 = 37.9, p < 0.001), the frequency of stops (LM df = 2, F = 12.25, p < 0.001) and duration of stops (Kruskal–Wallis df = 2, χ2 = 42.8, p < 0.001) all differed significantly between zones (Fig. 6). Based on post hoc analyses, the locomotor behaviour of V. destructor in the bubble zone was significantly different from its behaviour in the two other zones (Table 2).
Figure 6 Behavioural analyses of V. destructor females (N = 32) maintained in artificial arenas with a PBS bubble and a PBS discoid membrane. The first three boxplots show the ratios of the proportion of time spent (in %), the number of stops, the duration of stops (in seconds) weighed by the surface of each zone of the arena (in cm2). The frequency of pulse (/min) was also analysed by zone and is displayed on the fourth boxplot. The diamond points show the mean value. The four parameters all differed significantly between the zones considered (Table 2). |
Post hoc analyses of the behaviours of V. destructor in experimental arenas (N = 32).
When the females V. destructor are immobile on a surface, they can often be seen pulsating as described by Hall et al. (2021; Movie S1). The frequency of these pulses was higher in the bubble zone (22.9 ± 3.1 pulses/min) when compared to the other two zones (0.9 ± 0.7 pulses/min on the membrane and 6.5 ± 1.7 pulses/min in the rest of the arena). In the rest of the arena, most of the pulses (78%) occurred on the Parafilm walls. The pulses occurred significantly more often on the bubble than expected if the probability of pulsing would be randomly expressed, in which case it would depend only on the time spent in the zone (Table 2, Fig. 6).
Discussion
It is well known that in a host-parasite relationship, the parasite will seek cues from its host to find and join it [8]. In its natural environment, the behaviour of the eyeless parasite V. destructor is highly influenced by semiochemicals emitted by honey bees [35]. Previous studies exploring the effect of olfaction on parasite feeding behaviour have highlighted their key role in the parasite lifecycle [38, 42]. Similarly, in artificial feeding experiments without honey bees, host kairomones were thought to be important for parasite orientation. Usually, the rubbing of a bee on the feeding site was thus recommended to leave useful olfactory cues and guide V. destructor towards the food source [50].
Our data suggest that in artificial conditions, olfactory cues from bees may not be essential for the parasite to find a food source. Artificial feeding of V. destructor foundresses can be achieved with about 90% success in the laboratory, without a need for chemical cues. This puts into perspective the crucial importance of honey bee olfactory cues, at least in laboratory conditions.
In the absence of honey bee kairomones, V. destructor could still rely on other sources of odours to detect the food source. Olfactory cues emitted by the feeding medium and/or the Parafilm membrane could for instance be used for the parasite orientation. In this work, we showed that a feeding medium containing only PBS can lead to a feeding success in 96% of the cases. Similarly, when the parasite is surrounded by Parafilm, its ability to find the specific feeding site remains high (around 93%). The significantly higher survival rate of females kept on a PBS medium when compared to unfed females is also interesting as it could be a convenient negative control in future laboratory rearing experiments. More strikingly, when deprived from its main olfactory organ (i.e., the sensory pit on the forelegs) [10], the parasite is still able to find the food source and feed through the membrane. By preventing V. destructor from relying on environmental odours to forage, we provide evidence of artificial feeding with no or a limited role of olfaction. If the parasite is able to locate a food source without the help of olfactory cues, the process underlying this capacity remains an enigma.
In some liquid sucking parasitic insects such as aphids, it is known that foraging depends on a trial-and-error (probing) strategy [47, 59]. In V. destructor, the searching of the feeding site in natural conditions seems much more oriented and the female does not appear to bite randomly. When it feeds on pupae, a single feeding site located on the ventral side of the abdomen is made [11, 26]. On adults, several feeding sites are observed but they are also concentrated on the ventro-lateral side of the abdomen [50]. In artificial conditions, although a probing behaviour cannot be ruled out, the parasite appears more likely to rely on non-olfactory tactile or physical cues. Thanks to thermo and hygroreceptors located on the forelegs, it is known that V. destructor is able to detect humidity or heat [10, 29]. The existence of other receptors and the exact role of the foreleg sensilla in the process, however, remain unclear. In our experiments, temperature was kept constant in the gelatine capsule and the parasite’s orientation did not appear to depend on it. Regarding humidity, Parafilm is considered a waterproof membrane and an alternative source of humidity does not affect the mite’s feeding success. Furthermore, our behavioural bioassay where parasites discriminated between two similar membranes covering the same PBS solution shows that humidity is not an essential factor in this context. In contrast, our results provide support for the hypothesis that the parasite uses physical cues to locate food sources. During our 10-minute behavioural tests, the shape of surfaces seems to be recognised by V. destructor females as they are able to detect a 3D shaped membrane and explore it more extensively than the alternative 2D membrane. The time spent, number and duration of stops are all higher on the Parafilm bubble than in the rest of the arena, especially if we consider the reduced surface of the bubble. The Parafilm bubble thus seems to have an arrestant effect on the parasite. In this case, the parasite would first walk randomly in the arena and as the unexplored space is reduced, would finally find the food source. The arrestant effect of the 3D rounded aspect would lead V. destructor females to stay on this “larval like” shape. In arthropods, the tactile sense has rarely been studied in comparison with vision, olfaction or hearing, although it seems to be highly developed in several Arachnid species [3]. It relies on mechanoreceptors that are widely abundant on their hairy integument [3, 54, 64]. The tactile sense was shown to be essential for reproduction, orientation or hygiene in mites and spiders [4, 53–55]. In the two-spotted spider mite for instance, shape of quiescent deutonymphs seems to trigger the arrestment behaviour of reproductive males [53]. In our study, female V. destructor expressed the same type of arrestment when in contact with a rounded surface covering a liquid. The shape of the feeding site thus seems crucial. If it is not adequate or too flat, the female will spend less time exploring, which would result in an inability to feed. Since females explored the bubble more extensively than the 3D rounded walls of the arena, they could also have detected that the Parafilm membrane is thinner on the bubble and that it covers a liquid solution, which would make it more suitable for feeding.
In natural conditions, the ability to assess shapes and surfaces in dark and odour-saturated sealed brood could be beneficial for the mite to find its way around the cell and feed on the bee pupa. The behavioural mechanisms behind this capacity, however, remain unknown. Hall et al. (2022) recently described an unknown pulsating behaviour expressed by V. destructor females when they are exploring their environment [19]. We also observed this pulsating behaviour (Movie S1) and quantified it. We highlighted a higher frequency on 3D membrane compared to the rest of the arena, even when the total amount of time spent in each zone was taken into account. This means that once on the membrane, V. destructor spent less time moving and more time pulsating. The fact that pulsations are more expressed on liquid covering curved membranes could indicate that it is a crucial behaviour making it possible to gather physical cues from the environment, at least in the absence of olfactory cues. Further experiments are needed to examine whether V. destructor expresses this vibrational behaviour to assess the suitability of the feeding site before engaging its feeding behaviour [30, 48].
In any case, V. destructor would not be an exception as many other arthropods are known to vibrate. Biotremology is indeed a whole field in biology that highlights the importance of multimodal signals in animal behaviour, with a particular attention to the neglected aspect of vibrational communication and orientation [21]. Again, many arachnid species are well known to use vibrations in their foraging [5, 15, 30, 61]. Scorpions even possess an anatomical structure, namely the basitarsal compound slit sensilla that was shown to act as a vibrational receptor [67]. To our knowledge however, no similar structure has been described in V. destructor but it would be interesting to explore it further.
Altogether, our results obtained in an artificial context put into perspective the relative importance of olfactory and tactile senses in V. destructor. Whether the parasite’s ability to detect different shapes is always expressed or only in critical conditions remains to be determined. The artificial feeding method offers a controlled environment which allows for the exploration of hypotheses that are difficult to test in natural conditions. Unexplored aspects of parasite biology, like its nutritional requirements and foraging behaviour, could indeed be investigated through laboratory experiments. The consistent ingestion of artificial food from 3D Parafilm dummies is also a first step in the development of a long-term rearing method for V. destructor, using synthetic or semi-natural feeding solutions. However, if nutrition can be triggered with only a few or no olfactory cues, this is unlikely to be the case for reproduction, and recreating the whole parasite cycle in laboratory conditions remains the next big challenge.
Acknowledgments
We are thankful to ADA Occitanie for the biological material and to Quentin Maury for his help designing the arenas. No competing interests to declare.
Supplementary materials
Figure S1: Effect of an alternative source of humidity on the ability of Varroa destructor to find the food source. |
Figure S2: The behavioural bioassay arena from above. |
Video S1-a: Observation of Varroa destructor females’ pulsating behaviour under a stereomicroscope – dorsal view (×10). Access here
Video S1-b: Observation of Varroa destructor females’ pulsating behaviour under a stereomicroscope – lateral view (×40). Access here
Video S1-ci: Observation of Varroa destructor females’ pulsating behaviour under a stereomicroscope – ventral view (×20) Access here
Funding
This research was funded by Region Occitanie (ADEME grant No. 2082C0061).
References
- Anderson JF, Magnarelli LA. 2008. Biology of Ticks. Infectious Disease Clinics of North America, 22, 195–215. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azandémè-Hounmalon GY, Torto B, Fiaboe KKM, Subramanian S, Kreiter S, Martin T. 2016. Visual, vibratory, and olfactory cues affect interactions between the red spider mite Tetranychus evansi and its predator Phytoseiulus longipes. Journal of Pest Science, 89, 137–152. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Barth FG. 2016. A spider’s sense of touch: What to do with Myriads of tactile hairs? in The ecology of animal senses: matched filters for econical senses. Von-Der-Emde G, Warrant E, Editors. Springer International Publishing: Cham. p. 27–57. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Barth FG, Németh SS, Friedrich OC. 2004. Arthropod touch reception: Structure and mechanics of the basal part of a spider tactile hair. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 190, 523–530. [Google Scholar]
- Brownell PH. 1977. Compressional and surface waves in sand: Used by desert scorpions to locate prey. Science, 197, 479–482. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calderone NW, Lin S. 2001. Behavioural responses of Varroa destructor (Acari: Varroidae) to extracts of larvae, cocoons and brood food of worker and drone honey bees, Apis mellifera (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Physiological Entomology, 26, 341–350. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Calvo FJ, Knapp M, van Houten YM, Hoogerbrugge H, Belda JE. 2015. Amblyseius swirskii: What made this predatory mite such a successful biocontrol agent? Experimental and Applied Acarology, 65, 419–433. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaisson KE, Hallem EA. 2012. Chemosensory behaviors of parasites. Trends in Parasitology, 28, 427–436. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dicke M, Sabelis MW. 1988. How plants obtain predatory mites as bodyguards. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 38, 148–165. [Google Scholar]
- Dillier FX, Fluri P, Imdorf A. 2006. Review of the orientation behaviour in the bee parasitic mite Varroa destructor: Sensory equipment and cell invasion behaviour. Revue Suisse de Zoologie, 113, 857–877. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Donzé G, Guerin PM. 1994. Behavioral attributes and parental care of Varroa mites parasitizing honeybee brood. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 34, 305–319. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Donzé G, Schnyder-Candrian S, Bogdanov S, Diehl PA, Guerin PM, Kilchenman V, Monachon F. 1998. Aliphatic alcohols and aldehydes of the honey bee cocoon induce arrestment behavior in Varroa jacobsoni (Acari: Mesostigmata), an ectoparasite of Apis mellifera. Archives of Insect Biochemistry and Physiology, 37, 129–145. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Egan ME, Barth RH, Hanson FE. 1975. Chemically-mediated host selection in a parasitic mite. Nature, 257, 739. [Google Scholar]
- Fain A. 1994. Adaptation, specificity and host-parasite coevolution in mites (Acari). International Journal for Parasitology, 24, 1273–1283. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finck A. 1981. The lyriform organ of the orb-weaving spider Araneous sericatus: Vibration sensitivity is altered by bending the leg. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 70, 231–233. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Frey E, Odemer R, Blum T, Rosenkranz P. 2013. Activation and interruption of the reproduction of Varroa destructor is triggered by host signals (Apis mellifera). Journal of Invertebrate Pathology, 113, 56–62. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gherman CM, Mihalca AD, Dumitrache MO, Györke A, Oroian I, Sandor M, Cozma V. 2012. CO2 flagging – An improved method for the collection of questing ticks. Parasites & Vectors, 5, 1–7. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halbritter DA, Mullens BA. 2011. Responses of Ornithonyssus sylviarum (Acari: Macronyssidae) and Menacanthus stramineus (Phthiraptera: Menoponidae) to gradients of temperature, light, and humidity, with comments on microhabitat selection on chickens. Journal of Medical Entomology, 48, 251–261. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall H, Bencsik M, Newton MI, Chandler D, Prince G, Dwyer S. 2022. Varroa destructor mites regularly generate ultra-short, high magnitude vibrational pulses. Entomologia Generalis, 42, 375–388. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Häußermann CK, Ziegelmann B, Bergmann P, Rosenkranz P. 2015. Male mites (Varroa destructor) perceive the female sex pheromone with the sensory pit organ on the front leg tarsi. Apidologie, 46, 771–778. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Hill PSM, Lakes-Harlan R, Mazzoni V, Narins PM, Virant-Dorbelet M, Wessel A. 2019. Biotremology: Studying Vibrational Behavior. Springer International Publishing: Cham. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Horn CJ, Mierzejewski MK, Luong LT. 2018. Host respiration rate and injury-derived cues drive host preference by an ectoparasite of fruit flies. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology, 91, 896–903. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoy MA. 2016. Agricultural acarology: Introduction to integrated mite management. Taylor & F Taylor & Francis: Boca Raton. [Google Scholar]
- Hunter PE, Rosario RMT. 1988. Associations of Mesostigma with other arthropods. Annual Review of Entomology, 33, 393–417. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Jalil M, Rodriguez JG. 1970. Studies of behavior of Macrocheles muscaedomesticae (Acarina: macrochelidae) with emphasis on its attraction to the house fly. Annals of the Entomological Society of America, 63, 738–744. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanbar G, Engels W. 2004. Number and position of wounds on honey bee (Apis mellifera) pupae infested with a single Varroa mite. Development, 101, 323–326. [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy JS. 1978. The concepts of olfactory “arrestment” and “attraction”. Physiological Entomology, 3, 91–98. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Le Conte Y, Arnold G, Trouiller J, Masson C, Chappe B, Ourisson G. 1989. Attraction of the parasitic mite Varroa to the drone larvae of honey bees by simple aliphatic esters. Science, 245, 638–639. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Conte Y, Arnold G. 1988. Étude du thermopréférendum de Varroa jacobsoni Oud. Apidologie, 19, 155–164. [CrossRef] [EDP Sciences] [Google Scholar]
- Li AY, Cook SC, Sonenshine DE, Posada-Florez F, Noble NII, Mowery J, Gulbronson CJ, Bauchan GR. 2019. Insights into the feeding behaviors and biomechanics of Varroa destructor mites on honey bee pupae using electropenetrography and histology. Journal of Insect Physiology, 119, 103950. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maggi M, Damiani N, Ruffinengo S, De Jong D, Principal J, Eguaras M. 2010. Brood cell size of Apis mellifera modifies the reproductive behavior of Varroa destructor. Experimental Applied Acarology, 50, 269–279. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Migeon A, Nouguier E, Dorkeld F. 2010. Spider Mites Web: A comprehensive database for the Tetranychidae, in Trends in Acarology. Sabelis M, Bruin J, Editors. Springer: Dordrecht. p. 557–560. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Migeon A, Tixier M, Navajas M, Litskas VD, Stavrinides MC. 2019. A predator-prey system: Phytoseiulus persimilis (Acari: Phytoseiidae) and Tetranychus urticae (Acari: Tetranychidae): worldwide occurrence datasets. Acarologia, 59, 301–307. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Muleta MG, Schausberger P. 2013. Smells familiar: Group-joining decisions of predatory mites are mediated by olfactory cues of social familiarity. Animal Behaviour, 86, 507–512. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nazzi F, Le Conte Y. 2016. Ecology of Varroa destructor, the major ectoparasite of the western honey bee, Apis mellifera. Annual Review of Entomology, 61, 417–432. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nazzi F, Milani N, Della Vedova G. 2004. A semiochemical from larval food influences the entrance of Varroa destructor into brood cells. Apidologie, 35, 403–410. [CrossRef] [EDP Sciences] [Google Scholar]
- Neumann P, Carreck NL. 2010. Honey bee colony losses. Journal of Apicultural Research, 49, 1–6. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Nganso BT, Mani K, Altman Y, Rafaeli A, Soroker V. 2020. How crucial is the functional pit organ for the Varroa mite? Insects, 11, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
- Niogret J, Lumaret JP, Bertrand M. 2006. Semiochemicals mediating host-finding behaviour in the phoretic association between Macrocheles saceri (Acari: Mesostigmata) and Scarabaeus species (Coleoptera: Scarabaeidae). Chemoecology, 16, 129–134. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Noldus LPJJ, Spink AJ, Tegelenbosch RAJ. 2001. EthoVision: A versatile video tracking system for automation of behavioral experiments. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 33, 398–414. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen JP, Mullens BA. 2004. Influence of heat and vibration on the movement of the northern fowl mite (Acari: Macronyssidae). Journal of Medical Entomology, 41, 865–872. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pernal SF, Baird DS, Birmingham AL, Higo HA, Slessor KN, Winston ML. 2005. Semiochemicals influencing the host-finding behaviour of Varroa destructor. Experimental Applied Acarology, 37, 1–26. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piou V, Urrutia V, Laffont C, Hemptinne JL, Vétillard A. 2019. The nature of the arena surface affects the outcome of host-finding behavior bioassays in Varroa destructor (Anderson & Trueman). Parasitology Research, 118, 2935–2943. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piou V, Vilarem C, Rein C, Sprau L, Vétillard A. 2022. Standard methods for dissection of Varroa destructor Females. Insects, 13, 37. [Google Scholar]
- Plettner E, Eliash N, Singh NK, Pinnelli GR, Soroker V. 2017. The chemical ecology of host-parasite interaction as a target of Varroa destructor control agents. Apidologie, 48, 78–92. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Posada-Florez F, Ryabov E, Heerman MC, Chen Y, Evans JD, Sonenshine DE, Cook SC. 2020. Varroa destructor mites vector and transmit pathogenic honey bee viruses acquired from an artificial diet. Plos One, 15, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Powell G, Tosh CR, Hardie J. 2006. Host plant selection by aphids: Behavioral, evolutionary, and applied perspectives. Annual Review of Entomology, 51, 309–330. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin Y, Zhao J, Zhou L, Huang ZY. 2006. Electronic monitoring of feeding behaviour of Varroa mites on honey bees. Journal of Apicultural Research, 45, 157–158. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- R Core team. 2021. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria. [Google Scholar]
- Ramsey SD, Ochoa R, Bauchan G, Gulbronson C, Mowery JD, Cohen A, Lim D, Joklik J, Cicero JM, Ellis JD, Hawthorne D, Engelsdorp D Van. 2019. Varroa destructor feeds primarily on honey bee fat body tissue and not hemolymph. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116, 1792–1801. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rickli M, Guerin PM, Diehl PA. 1992. Palmitic acid released from honeybee worker larvae attracts the parasitic mite Varroa jacobsoni on a servosphere. Naturwissenschaften, 79, 320–322. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenkranz P, Aumeier P, Ziegelmann B. 2010. Biology and control of Varroa destructor. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology, 103(Suppl), S96–S119. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royalty RN, Phelan PL, Hall FR. 1993. Comparative effects of form, colour, and pheromone of twospotted spider mite quiescent deutonymphs on male guarding behaviour. Physiological Entomology, 18, 303–316. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Santer RD, Hebets EA. 2009. Tactile learning by a whip spider, Phrynus marginemaculatus C.L. Koch (Arachnida, Amblypygi). Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 195, 393–399. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato Y, Saito Y. 2006. Nest sanitation in social spider mites: Interspecific differences in defecation behavior. Ethology, 112, 664–669. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Su Y, Zhang B, Xu X. 2021. Chemosensory systems in predatory mites: From ecology to genome. Systematic and Applied Acarology, 26, 852–865. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Tabart J, Colin ME, Carayon JL, Tene N, Payre B, Vetillard A. 2013. Artificial feeding of Varroa destructor through a chitosan membrane: A tool for studying the host-microparasite relationship. Experimental and Applied Acarology, 61, 107–118. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takabayashi J, Dicke M. 1996. Plant-carnivore mutualism through herbivore-induced carnivore attractants. Trends in Plant Science, 1, 109–113. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Tjallingii WF. 1994. Sieve element acceptance by aphids. European Journal of Entomology, 91, 47–52. [Google Scholar]
- Traynor KS, Mondet F, de Miranda JR, Techer M, Kowallik V, Oddie MAY, Chantawannakul P, McAfee A. 2020. Varroa destructor: A complex parasite, crippling honey bees worldwide. Trends in Parasitology, 36, 592–606. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uetz GW, Roberts JA. 2002. Multisensory cues and multimodal communication in spiders: Insights from video/audio playback studies. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 59, 222–230. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vervaet L, De Vis R, De Clercq P, Van Leeuwen T. 2021. Is the emerging mite pest Aculops lycopersici controllable? Global and genome-based insights in its biology and management. Pest Management Science, 77, 2635–2644. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilarem C, Piou V, Vogelweith F, Vétillard A. 2021. Varroa destructor from the laboratory to the field: Control, biocontrol and IPM perspectives – a review. Insects, 12, 800. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waladde SM, Young AS, Morzaria SP. 1996. Artificial feeding of ixodid ticks. Parasitology Today, 12, 272–278. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Waladde SM, Rice MJ. 1982. The Sensory Basis of Tick Feeding Behaviour, in Physiol. Ticks. Obenchain FD, Galun R, Editors. Elsevier, Pergamon Press Ltd: Amsterdam. p. 71–118. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Walter DE, Proctor HC. 1999. Mites : Ecology, Evolution & Behaviour. Springer International Publishing: Dordrecht. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Wang K, Zhang J, Liu L, Chen D, Song H, Wang Y, Niu S, Han Z, Ren L. 2019. Vibrational receptor of scorpion (Heterometrus petersii): The basitarsal compound slit sensilla. Journal of Bionic Engineering, 16, 76–87. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. 2016. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer International Publishing: Cham. [Google Scholar]
- Van Wijk M, Wadman WJ, Sabelis MW. 2006. Morphology of the olfactory system in the predatory mite Phytoseiulus persimilis. Experimental and Applied Acarology, 40, 217–229. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoder JA, Pekins PJ, Lorenz AL, Nelson BW. 2017. Larval behaviour of the winter tick, Dermacentor albipictus (Acari:Ixodidae): evaluation of CO2 (dry ice), and short- and long-range attractants by bioassay. International Journal of Acarology, 43, 187–193. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
Cite this article as: Piou V, Vilarem C, Blanchard S, Armengaud C, Heeb P & Vétillard A. 2023. Varroa destructor relies on physical cues to feed in artificial conditions. Parasite 30, 49.
All Tables
Experimental groups from the artificial feeding experiment with parameters tested and sample sizes. The sample sizes are higher for three of the groups because they were used to assess the survival rates of the parasite.
Post hoc analyses of the behaviours of V. destructor in experimental arenas (N = 32).
All Figures
Figure 1 Experimental set up used in the artificial feeding experiments. The feeding chambers are made from a gelatine capsule and Parafilm™ membranes. FCF blue dye is included in the nutritive solution to allow for the observation of V. destructor feeding status. |
|
In the text |
Figure 2 Steps followed to make the experimental arena used in the behavioural bioassays. The 13 × 7 × 2 mm arena is covered with Parafilm. A bubble and a discoid membrane covered well are formed. Both contain the same Blue FCF dyed PBS solution. The tested female V. destructor is placed in the arena between the second and the third step, right before the start of the recording. |
|
In the text |
Figure 3 Feeding success of artificially fed V. destructor females. Percentage and 95% conf. interval of females that had fed 24 h after the start of the experiment, in relation to the experimental set up used (Control feeding (n = 140), Control feeding w/o rub (n = 32) PBS (n = 94) PBS + Parafilm (n = 28). Letters indicate the statistical significance between groups. |
|
In the text |
Figure 4 Survival of V. destructor females in artificial conditions. Survival rates and 95% conf. interval of females (in %) 24 h and 48 h after the start of the experiment, in relation to the experimental set up used (Control feeding n = 140; PBS n = 94; Negative control n = 82). Letters indicate the statistical significance between groups. |
|
In the text |
Figure 5 Effect of sensory pit organ deprivation on the ability of V. destructor to find a food source in artificial conditions. The graph shows the percentage and 95% conf. interval of varnished (n = 44) and control (n = 37) parasites that fed after 24 h in artificial conditions. Letters indicate the statistical significance between the two groups. |
|
In the text |
Figure 6 Behavioural analyses of V. destructor females (N = 32) maintained in artificial arenas with a PBS bubble and a PBS discoid membrane. The first three boxplots show the ratios of the proportion of time spent (in %), the number of stops, the duration of stops (in seconds) weighed by the surface of each zone of the arena (in cm2). The frequency of pulse (/min) was also analysed by zone and is displayed on the fourth boxplot. The diamond points show the mean value. The four parameters all differed significantly between the zones considered (Table 2). |
|
In the text |
Figure S1: Effect of an alternative source of humidity on the ability of Varroa destructor to find the food source. |
|
In the text |
Figure S2: The behavioural bioassay arena from above. |
|
In the text |
Current usage metrics show cumulative count of Article Views (full-text article views including HTML views, PDF and ePub downloads, according to the available data) and Abstracts Views on Vision4Press platform.
Data correspond to usage on the plateform after 2015. The current usage metrics is available 48-96 hours after online publication and is updated daily on week days.
Initial download of the metrics may take a while.