Issue
Parasite
Volume 21, 2014
Novel Approaches to the Control of Parasites in Goats and Sheep. Invited editors: Hervé Hoste, Smaragda Sotiraki and Michel Alvinerie
Article Number 31
Number of page(s) 10
DOI https://doi.org/10.1051/parasite/2014032
Published online 30 June 2014

© J.J. Villalba et al., published by EDP Sciences, 2014

Licence Creative CommonsThis is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1. Introduction

A herbivore’s existence is closely bound to that of parasites and pathogens [37]. Parasitism is a persistent challenge to their survival and reproduction [4047], causing negative impacts on their health, daily activities [37], well-being [78], and nutritional state [13]. Collectively, all these effects lead to significant reductions in herbivore fitness [74]. To combat parasitic infections, herbivores generate immune responses that both disrupt parasite establishment and/or development [13, 19] and enhance their ability to cope and maintain productivity in response to a parasitic challenge [1]. In addition, ruminants have evolved behavioral means to suppress (or prevent) the negative impacts of parasitism. For instance, grazing ruminants minimize the chances of infection by avoiding areas where parasite larvae are most concentrated [19, 39, 40]. Horses grazing in highly stocked pastures exhibit “latrine behavior” (i.e., defecate in restricted areas), whereas their counterparts grazing in rangelands, where the parasite risk is lower, defecate randomly while grazing [55].

Another behavioral mechanism for minimizing parasite infection is to self-select foods containing compounds that help treat or control parasite infections. This field of study is referred to as zoopharmacognosy or self-medication [33, 36]. It is based on the postulate that animals have evolved behavioral and physiological mechanisms to treat or control disease and/or its symptoms, leading to an improvement in health and, as a consequence, an enhancement in fitness [37]. In a seminal study, Huffman and Seifu [35] observed that wild chimpanzees suffering from parasite-related diseases consumed the bitter pith of the plant Vernonia amygdalina which contains sesquiterpene lactones and steroid glucosides – compounds with antiparasitic activity at the doses consumed by the animals [49]. Since these pioneering results, additional evidence pointing to the use of plant secondary compounds to recover or maintain health has been reported not only for chimps and other mammals [30], but also for insects [74] and birds [12]. The study of self-medication seeks to understand (a) how animals respond behaviorally to challenges that potentially affect their health, and (b) how these behaviors are maintained within a population [37].

In this paper we present evidence of self-medication in ruminant animals and propose mechanisms by which self-medicative behaviors are acquired. Our primary focus is self-medication in the context of ruminant animals infected with gastrointestinal endoparasites (primarily nematodes), and our aim is to stimulate further research aimed at understanding the underlying mechanisms and ontogeny of self-medicative behaviors. A deeper understanding of the mechanisms and triggers of self-medication by parasitized animals should pave the way to innovative management strategies to improve animal health and welfare in a sustainable way with reduced human interventions.

2. Evidence of self-medication in parasitized ruminants

Self-medicative behaviors, whereby plants containing natural anthelmintics are selectively ingested by ruminants, can be classified into two categories: prophylactic and therapeutic [32]. Goat kids from the Mamber breed, which generally exhibit low propensity to consume the antiparasitic shrub Pistacia lentiscus [56], increased their preference for this plant following infection with 10,000 L3 larvae of mixed gastrointestinal nematodes, suggesting therapeutic self-medication [2]. In contrast, goat kids from the Damascus breed typically ingest high amounts of the aforementioned plant irrespective of infection [25], thus exhibiting prophylactic self-medication. Prophylactic ingestion of medicinal plants encompasses behaviors that are likely rooted in fixed action patterns and genetic adaptations which are not necessarily linked to the current physiological state of an animal. While therapeutic self-medication emerges from a learning process involving the interaction between the orosensorial characteristics of foods and their post-ingestive medicinal effects (i.e., a feedback mechanism), prophylactic self-medication can be explained through a preventive “feedforward” mechanism [89]. The present review will focus on the emergence of functional behavioral responses to the challenges imposed by endoparasites with an emphasis on therapeutic self-medication.

Surveys and field observations, as well as controlled studies, indicate that parasitized ruminants exhibit self-medication. For instance, surveys have revealed that parasitized goats self-select unpalatable plants with anthelmintic properties [30] and ingest higher percentages of heather containing tannin (an anthelmintic plant secondary compound) than anthelmintic-treated goats [64]. Likewise, sheep infected with adult populations of Haemonchus contortus eat more of the tannin-rich plant Lysiloma latisiliquum (Tzalam) than non-infected animals [59].

In a controlled experiment, lambs experiencing natural gastrointestinal helminth burdens ate more of a tannin-rich supplement than non-parasitized animals, even when the supplement was of very low nutritional value. In contrast, non-parasitized lambs consumed more of the supplement without tannins than parasitized lambs [58]. Likewise, lambs with natural gastrointestinal parasitic burdens and non-parasitized lambs were offered a choice of alfalfa (Medicago sativa) and a mix containing 90% alfalfa and 10% quebracho tannin. Parasitized lambs showed a greater preference for the tannin-containing food than non-parasitized animals; these differences disappeared when parasite loads were eliminated by chemotherapy [86].

Previous experience and learning play a role in self-medication. For instance, when lambs were infected with 10,000 L3 (infective larvae) of H. contortus, intake of and preference for a tannin-rich feed was high for lambs that had previously experienced the beneficial effects of condensed tannins while parasitized; intake was comparatively low in lambs lacking this experience. When parasitic infections were terminated by chemotherapy, differences between groups disappeared [43]. This suggests that individual experience with the medicinal effects of tannins enhances intake of and preference for tannins during subsequent parasite infections.

3. A functional explanation for self-medication in ruminants

The senses of smell, taste, mouthfeel, and sight enable animals to discriminate among foods. Post-ingestive feedback calibrates these sensory experiences – like or dislike – in accord with a food’s positive or negative post-oral effects [69]. Such dynamic integration of chemosensory and post-ingestive signals gives ruminants the ability to modify diet selection as a function of the consequences of food experienced throughout their lifetimes [71].

Nutritional state and dietary experience in ruminants can modify ingestive responses to foods. Lambs fed diets low in energy and protein prefer flavored foods previously paired with intra-ruminal infusions of energy (starch, propionate, acetate) or nitrogen (urea, casein, gluten) [8285]. From a functional standpoint, ingesting medicine is not so different from acquiring nutrients while foraging. If herbivores can learn to prefer nutritious foods, they may also learn to prefer medicinally beneficial foods [43].

The most significant effect of gastrointestinal parasites is a depression in food intake [76], which is partially attributed to pain and discomfort associated with the infection as well as to hormonal changes from disrupted gastrointestinal function [77]. Helminths damage gut tissue and impair mucoprotein secretion [8, 75]. For instance, the larvae of Trichostrongylus in the small intestine may cause severe damage to the intestinal mucous membrane, and the L4 larval stage and adults of Haemonchus are blood feeders that cause anemia and disrupt mucous membranes in the abomasum [15]. In addition, it has been suggested that the cytokine release that accompanies infection reduces food intake [57]. All these effects can cause discomfort and gastrointestinal distress, which decreases food palatability and increases the formation of acquired distastes [67] (Fig. 1). Thus, it is expected that parasitized animals will readily acquire a distaste for a non-medicinal diet. Consistent with this expectation, parasitized sheep exhibit a low preference for the non-medicinal legume cicer milkvetch relative to a baseline period before infection [89]. On the other hand, animals should be able to learn about the medicinal benefits of a food and acquire a preference for that food when its ingestion is associated with relief from illness or discomfort [88]. In support of this possibility, reductions in gastrointestinal distress have been found to increase food palatability and condition preferences in rats [67]. Herbivores may also be able to develop a conditioned preference for foods associated with recovery from gastrointestinal distress [88] (Fig. 1). This may explain why parasitized lambs exhibit a higher preference for the tannin-containing legume sainfoin than non-parasitized lambs [89].

thumbnail Figure 1.

(A) Gastrointestinal parasites cause pain and discomfort. Non-medicinal foods associated with such sensations may lead to an acquired distaste. (B) In contrast, medicinal antiparasitic foods that promote relief can lead to a preference, i.e., self-medication.

It has been proposed that post-ingestive consequences (e.g., illness) from a food must occur shortly after feeding in order for ruminants to condition an aversion to that food [91]. However, more recent research has shown that ruminants can condition aversions to toxic foods following delays of up to 8 h between food ingestion and consequences [10], and can condition preferences for calorically rich foods following delays of up to 1 h [86]. Relief from parasitism after medicinal plant ingestion may commence within 1–8 h [61], suggesting that this time delay will not prevent herbivores from learning about the positive post-ingestive effects of medicinal plants on parasitic burdens.

Nevertheless, some goat breeds exhibit high propensity to consume an anthelmintic tannin-rich food even when they are naïve to it and even when they do not experience a worm load concomitantly to ingesting tannins [2]. This observation raises the possibility that whereas some ruminants must condition preferences for foods with anthelmintic properties, others possess innate preferences for them.

4. The triggers for self-medication in ruminants

From the previous analysis, we can infer that ruminants need to ingest doses of an antiparasitic medicine (i.e., a plant secondary compound) in order to experience its benefits and thus learn to self-medicate. Indeed, Mediterranean goats in tannin-rich environments will typically ingest 1 g/kg BW of PEG-binding tannins [25], which is the dose needed to suppress fecal excretion of nematode eggs almost entirely [56]. However, many antiparasitic medicines found in nature are secondary compounds, which evolved to deter herbivory by inducing negative post-ingestive effects [66]. In addition, many antiparasitic secondary compounds have a bitter taste. Garcia and Hankins [21] maintained that mammals should avoid anything that tastes unpalatable, particularly if it is bitter. This rule is supported by the observations that virtually all naturally occurring poisons taste bitter to humans [28], and most chemicals that taste bitter to humans also elicit an aversive response in other mammals [27]. In addition, many plant secondary compounds deter herbivory by eliciting burning, sour and astringent oral sensations. Some others also produce unpleasant odors or irritating sensations in the nose [27, 28]. Thus, secondary compounds provide unpleasant oral and nasal sensations, which deter feeding by herbivores.

What mechanisms allow parasitized ruminants to ingest therapeutic doses of potentially toxic chemicals that also induce unpleasant taste and odor sensations? It is known that ruminants change their behavior in response to parasitism. We will review some of these changes and propose some others in an attempt to answer this question.

4.1 Parasitism and changes in feeding behavior

Several behavioral changes caused by endoparasites are rooted in the induction of anorexia. Anorexia is a common symptom of parasitic infectious diseases, but one that has been qualified as paradoxical because parasites typically impose increased metabolic and nutritional demands on the host [50, 54]. Anorexia is manifested in parasitized ruminants as shorter and fewer feeding bouts relative to non-infected individuals [1618]. In addition, the extent, duration and rate of recovery of pathogen-induced anorexia are influenced by the type of food available. Parasitized individuals consuming a nutritious food are anorexic for a shorter period of time than individuals exposed to a food of lower nutritional quality [54].

Rather than being a paradoxical response, anorexia may in fact represent a behavioral adaptation [6]. For instance, it has been hypothesized that anorexia allows the host to become more selective in its diet, and thus choose foods that either minimize the risk of infection or augment the intake of antiparasitic compounds [53]. For instance, when parasitized herbivores were offered a choice between non-contaminated and feces-contaminated pastures, they avoided the latter pastures [14, 39]. Avoidance of feces has been reported in non-parasitized animals, but this behavior appears to be more pronounced in parasitized animals [39]. The avoidance of feces by parasitized animals occurs even when feces-contaminated fields offer higher nutrient rewards [40]. When infected animals were forced to consume contaminated pastures, they grazed further from the soil surface than non-parasitized animals, thereby minimizing the risk of parasite intake [40].

There is also evidence for the hypothesis that parasitized animals become more selective. When sheep infected with larvae of the intestinal nematode Trichostrongylus colubriformis were given a choice between two feeds that differed in protein content, they exhibited unusually high preferences for the high-protein feed, likely to meet the increased protein requirements due to parasitism [51, 52]. However, an increase in protein selection was not observed in dairy heifers sub-clinically infected with Cooperia oncophora and Ostertagia ostertagi [18].

Endoparasites also affect other aspects of behavior. It has been shown that parasitized animals take fewer steps, lie longer and change posture less frequently than non-parasitized animals [78]. This reduced activity may be a consequence of lethargy likely aimed at conserving energy [31], or as another parasite-avoidance strategy [53].

Even when parasitism induces anorexia and lethargy, enhanced preferences for medicinal plants may nevertheless lead to an increased dose of antiparasitic agents consumed with the diet, despite the fact that overall food intake may decline. More work is needed to explore this possibility.

4.2 Neophilia

In addition to anorexia and avoidance behavior, parasitism may increase the attractiveness of novel foods and orosensory stimuli (i.e., lead to enhanced neophilia), and thereby increase the likelihood that therapeutic doses of prophylactic plant secondary compounds will be consumed (Fig. 2).

thumbnail Figure 2.

A conceptual representation of how pre- and post-ingestive events control the manifestation of self-medicative behavior in mammalian herbivores. Self-medication emerges from enhanced neophilia and increased acceptance of certain somatosensations (e.g., taste dimensions, tactile properties) triggered by parasitism. These increases in neophilia and acceptability, together with social learning, should “prime” animals to ingest therapeutic doses of medicinal secondary compounds (pre-ingestive processes). Subsequently, associative learning (i.e., associations between orosensorial properties of a medicinal food and relief experienced after ingesting that food) will maintain and/or reinforce self-medicative behaviors. Thus, a chain of events starting with food acceptability and social learning followed by post-ingestive processes may contribute to the emergence of self-medication in mammalian herbivores.

When physiological requirements are met and the animal is in a homeostatic state – such as when balanced rations are available ad libitum – ruminants eat only small amounts of novel foods. This neophobic behavior likely evolved to decrease the likelihood of consuming harmful foods. However, departure from homeostasis (e.g., when there is an inadequate or unbalanced supply of nutrients) may cause neophilia [70]. Likewise, birds on a positive energy budget are averse to risk, but prone to it when on a negative energy budget [11]. This is because individuals that have acquired abundant reserves of energy have more potential fitness to lose from taking risks (e.g., selecting novel foods, exploring new places) than individuals which have not [9]. Likewise, sick individuals may have less potential fitness to lose from taking risks (e.g., by selecting novel foods or foods containing novel plant secondary compounds) than healthy ones. Thus, one would expect that when homeostasis is disturbed by parasite infection (which imposes metabolic constraints), animals should become more neophilic. This prediction has been explored recently in our laboratories: parasitized goats increased their intake of tannin-rich P. lentiscus relative to non-parasitized counterparts if they had been conditioned before infection to hay or to another browse species, but not if they had been conditioned to P. lentiscus [2]. Also, we offered parasitized (H. contortus) and non-parasitized lambs a novel food (beet pulp) with increasing concentrations of condensed tannins. Lambs had a trinary choice of beet pulp containing 0%, 5%, and 10% quebracho tannins. First, consumption of the novel beet pulp with 0% tannins was greater in parasitized than non-parasitized animals. Second, during the initial 2 d of exposure, parasitized animals preferred the beet pulp without condensed tannins, but by day 6 they ingested a greater proportion of beet pulp adulterated with condensed tannins than non-parasitized lambs (Egea, Miller, Hall, and Villalba, unpublished results). Thus, parasitized lambs showed initially a greater degree of acceptance of the novel food with a taste salience presumably similar to the (familiar) basal diet of alfalfa pellets. For a consumer, taste salience depends on novelty of the taste not on concentration [46], and the taste of tannins was certainly more novel than that of beet pulp. Since beet pulp did not provide a medicinal effect, it is possible that on subsequent days parasitized lambs displayed an increased acceptance of the “more novel” tannin-containing foods, which provided a medicinal effect to the consumers. Alternatively, when rodents repeatedly sample tannins, they induce production of salivary proline-rich proteins (PRPs) which bind to the tannins and render them more palatable [26]. If the same induction process for tannin-binding salivary proteins occurs in lambs, as has been suggested in lambs exposed to tannin-rich forage [81], then this could explain the gradual increase in intake of the tannin-treated diet by parasitized animals, which repeatedly sampled such a diet.

Increased acceptance of certain taste sensations may substantially benefit herbivores. For instance, if herbivores could tolerate the taste of bitter foods, then they could substantially expand their range of dietary options (Fig. 2). They could also increase their intake of therapeutic secondary compounds, which typically taste bitter [27, 37]. Repeated sampling of the bitter-tasting anti-malarial agent chloroquine by malaria-infected mice resulted in significant reductions in parasitemia and risk of mortality [90]. Thus, there is evidence in mouse models that acceptance of bitter taste has the potential to enhance fitness when consumers are challenged by parasitism. This response can be explained by a preventive “feedforward” mechanism or prophylactic self-medication [90]. Feedforward mechanisms can also explain why there is a fine line between medicine and food in both primates and indigenous peoples in different parts of the world. In mountain gorillas, for instance, 30% of their daily herbaceous diet contains plant secondary compounds with antibacterial properties. Of the 172 plant species typically consumed by Mahale chimpanzees, 22% are used to treat gastrointestinal-related illnesses in humans. In addition, 89% of the species used to treat symptoms of malaria among the Hausa of Nigeria are also used in a dietary context [33]. Antioxidants such as flavonoids reduce the negative impacts of free radicals in the body and birds preferentially select flavonoids in their diets, which leads to lower oxidative stress and enhanced immunity, presumably through prophylactic self-medication [12]. Would feedforward mechanisms eventually prime animals to consume therapeutic doses of anthelmintics and thereby enhance their ability to learn about the medicinal post-ingestive effects of foods? The answer to this question may be affirmative, but more research is needed in this regard; we still do not know whether parasitized ruminants feed preferentially on anything that tastes bitter, unpleasant, or novel. If parasite infections cause animals to increase intake of unpleasant-tasting plant tissues, then what is the mechanism underlying this phenomenon? Does the food remain unpalatable, but the animal increases its tolerance for the orosensory properties of the food? Alternatively, does parasite infection make animals less sensitive to unpleasant-tasting plant tissues? In caterpillars, infection by parasites enhances acceptance of the taste of some antiparasitic pyrrolizidine alkaloids, which has a positive impact on fitness [7].

4.3 Social models

Social models play an important role in diet selection and preferences of young animals [20]. For instance, the degree of affinity of lambs with their social models affects their acceptability of novel foods [79]. Socializing enhances learning efficiency because each animal no longer has to discover everything through trial and error (e.g., after experiencing the positive or negative post-ingestive consequences of a novel food). Pioneering animals in a social group may learn about the beneficial effects of specific foods or combinations of foods. Once this pioneer develops a preference for a food, the preference may then spread through the group, becoming part of the foraging behavior of females [36], who can then transmit those behaviors to their offspring. Cultural transmission and maintenance of leaf swallowing (a self-medicative behavior directed to combat gastrointestinal endoparasites) within a group and subsequent associative learning by the individual of the positive consequences of this behavior has been proposed to be the pivotal link between the propensity for consuming leaves and its maintenance as a self-medicative behavior by great apes in the wild [38]. Such transmission of information across generations occurs in livestock. For instance, when offered a choice, lambs tend to show the same food preferences and aversions as their mothers [62, 63]. Mothers also appear to play a critical role in “teaching” naïve lambs self-medicative behaviors [72]. Mediterranean goats consume tannin-rich Pistacia lentiscus even at times of plentiful green herbage, and educate their kids to consume the plant in high (Damascus goats) or moderate amounts (Mamber goats) [24]. This could be the basis of self-medication behavior, as ingesting P. lentiscus foliage decreases the incidence of nematode eggs in feces to almost zero [56] and impairs development of gastrointestinal larvae [4]. By educating their kids to consume P. lentiscus moderately [24], Mamber does prepare the ground for therapeutically increased intake of this shrub in case of infection. In contrast, Damascus goats encourage high propensity to consume the shrub, i.e., educate their kids to self-medicate in a prophylactic way.

There is a trade-off between social information and competition: grazing in a group is more efficient, and animals spend less time evaluating which foods to eat and which foods to avoid. Grazing alone involves less competition with peers [73], but grazing in a group increases the odds of being infected by parasites.

Interestingly, in a Mediterranean rangeland dominated by tannin-rich Quercus coccifera, the most dominant goats consumed more shrubs than lower-ranked counterparts [5]. In other words, aggressiveness was not directed at consuming the most nutritious diet, but instead, at consuming a diet that protects consumers from parasites. A survey carried out in the Cazorla Natural Park of Spain [22] showed that wild goats (Capra pyrenaica) consumed a diet consisting of 41% browse and 59% herbaceous vegetation, whereas domestic goats (Capra hircus) selected a diet with 81% browse and 19% herbaceous vegetation; and the diets of wild sheep (Ovis musimon) contained 80% herbaceous species, compared with 48% for domestic sheep (Ovis aries), which also included 25% of dwarf shrubs in their diets. Collectively, the information presented suggests that it is likely that social models in domesticated ruminants “prime” the individual to increase consumption of an otherwise typically avoided secondary compound-containing food (Fig. 2).

5. Integration of pre- and post-ingestive events on self-medication

If a plant’s tissues contain a secondary compound that has anti-parasitic properties but does not confer unpalatability, then the likelihood of parasitized herbivores consuming the plant (and perhaps benefitting from its medicinal effects) would be high. In contrast, if another plant’s tissues contain a secondary compound that has antiparasitic properties but makes the plant unpalatable, then the likelihood of parasitized herbivores consuming this plant (and perhaps benefitting from its medicinal effects) would be comparatively low. Low or non-therapeutic doses of medicines should prevent animals from experiencing the potential medicinal benefits of such compounds, and thereby prevent the emergence of self-medicative behavior. However, if sick animals “cross the rejection threshold” and experience the benefits of consuming the medicine, they may continue ingesting the medicinal compounds and the behavior may persist in time with the potential to be transmitted within a social group. In this case the therapeutic benefits may outweigh the toxic effects, particularly for secondary compounds such as tannins, which have relatively low toxicity [64]. Thus, if the medicinal benefits of pharmaceutically active compounds overrule the toxic effects and increase fitness in parasitized individuals, then it is expected that consumers will “cross the rejection threshold” in such a scenario (e.g., [58, 74]). In some instances, herbivores may need to experience the post-ingestive effects of an unpalatable medicinal food in a conditioning session where no food alternatives are available, so this intervention would help the animal “cross the rejection threshold” imposed by the foods’ orosensorial properties.

In the wild, mammalian herbivores are capable of regulating their intake of secondary compounds. It has been proposed that this regulation occurs through post-oral mechanisms that influence the size of a meal and the inter-meal interval [80]. In brushland, free-ranging goats consume diets with 3%–3.5% of condensed tannins throughout the year, in spite of the extreme variability of browse and herbage on offer [45]. These mechanisms involve sensing secondary compounds in the gut and blood, as well as conditioned food aversions [29, 80]. The idea that ingestion of secondary compounds is a regulated process, which seeks to keep ingested secondary compounds at a subtoxic level, raises the possibility that these compounds may have a prophylactic action against intestinal parasites at the doses consumed by infected herbivores. Self-medication is also a regulated process (instead of an all-or-none process) in which animals incorporate certain sub-toxic doses of plant secondary compounds into their diets and sometimes for only some days during infection, instead of manifesting strong preferences for secondary compound-containing foods [35, 44].

6. Conclusions and future directions

Over the past 40 years, we have gained insight into how nutritional deficiencies alter feeding preferences in ruminants. We are just starting to understand how parasitic infections and illness alter the onset of self-selection of medicines in these animals. Emerging evidence suggests ruminants are able to self-medicate by ingesting plant tissues that contain pharmaceutically active compounds. However, less clear are the mechanisms by which self-medicative behaviors are initiated and evolve in ruminants. Even if self-medication can be attributed to a behavioral tradition, this leaves open questions about how the behavior was individually acquired in the first place and how individuals become predisposed to ingest medicinal plants [36]. A better understanding about the mechanisms that are involved in the learning process of self-medication will help managers devise innovative strategies that enhance the use of plants containing antiparasitic compounds.

Disease caused by gastrointestinal nematodes is one of the most important health constraints affecting productivity in small ruminants [42]. There is an increased interest in finding alternative treatments for parasite control due to the fact that pathogens are rapidly developing resistance to existing drugs [41]. For farm animals in particular, there is a clear desire to create systems of production that rely less heavily on chemotherapy [3]. Self-medication represents a sustainable and targeted strategy of parasite control [34] since animals can potentially select medicinal plants as a function of need. Self-medication also enhances the nutrition and welfare of ruminant animals as they do not need to be forced to consume a monoculture of a certain bioactive-containing plant; they can self-select the medicinal plant while they still have an array of nutritive alternatives available.

Despite the aforementioned benefits, more research is needed to understand the mechanisms that trigger and sustain the utilization of antiparasitic plants and supplements better. For instance, we need more knowledge on how ruminants experience malaise during a parasitic infection and how they experience relief after consuming an antiparasitic food (Fig. 1). How do animals identify medicinally active plants? When they get sick, do they simply feed preferentially on anything that tastes/smells unpleasant? Do they learn from other animals what to eat? Do they use a trial-and-error system? For instance, if parasitic infections cause animals to increase intake of unpleasant-tasting/smelling plant tissues, then what is the mechanism underlying this phenomenon? Does the food remain unpalatable, but the animal tolerates its bad taste/smell and eats it anyway? Alternatively, does the orosensation of a medicinal food become more acceptable during sickness?

It is known that some by-products of infection such as lipopolysaccharides, peptidoglycans, and microbial nucleic acids stimulate the production of proinflammatory cytokines, which reach the central nervous system and serve as endogenous mediators of anorexia [57]. Damage of gut tissue may lead to local inflammation [8, 75]. Gastrointestinal parasitism causes damage to the gastrointestinal mucosa, which results in increased plasma leakage and losses of endogenous protein to the lumen [68]. Endoparasites may also disrupt absorption and retention of nitrogen, minerals, and vitamins [23, 48]. Thus, central and peripheral mechanisms may trigger discomfort in the host. In turn, substances that block cytokine synthesis/action and/or reduce inflammatory responses or pain may lead to an alleviation of the discomfort experienced by parasitism and thus enhance liking for the food associated with this sensation.

More research is also needed for elucidating the mechanisms that trigger acceptance of antiparasitic substances by the host. It is likely that parasitism triggers the upregulation of specific taste receptors which in turn increase the acceptance of certain taste dimensions (i.e., bitter) and thus the increased intake of antiparasitic foods. In caterpillars, infection by parasites alters the taste response for specific medicinal plant secondary compounds (pyrrolizidine alkaloids) which encourages secondary compound ingestion, providing a biochemical defense [7]. Novel genes for bitter taste reception are being identified in ruminants [19] and this knowledge can contribute to designing experiments in parasitized and non-parasitized animals that lead to a better understanding of the triggers of self-medicative behavior in ruminants. Ingestion of bacterial lipopolysaccharides alters sweet taste function in mice [92]. These lipopolysaccharides seem to mediate the production of proinflammatory cytokines that downregulate sweet taste receptors genes in taste buds [92]. Thus, it is likely that some by-products of infection influence taste sensation in infected animals. Finally, more research is needed to establish the link between social transmission of information regarding diet selection and the ability of ruminants to self-medicate.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Utah Agricultural Experiment Station, Utah State University, and approved as Journal Paper No. 8660.

References

  1. Albers GAA, Gray GD, Piper LR, Barber JSF, LeJambre LF, Barger IA. 1987. The genetics of resistance and resilience to Haemonchus contortus infection in young Merino sheep. International Journal of Parasitology, 17, 1355–1367. [Google Scholar]
  2. Amit A, Cohen I, Marcovics A, Muklada H, Glasser TA, Ungar ED, Landau SY. 2013. Self-medication with tannin-rich browse in goats infectedwith gastro-intestinal nematodes. Veterinary Parasitology, 198, 305–311. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Athanasiadou S, Arsenos G, Kyriazakis I. 2002. Animal health and welfare issues arising in organic ruminant production systems, in Organic Meat and Milk from Ruminants. Kyriazakis I, Zervas G, Editors. Wageningen Academic Publishers. p. 39–56. [Google Scholar]
  4. Azaizeh H, Halahleh F, Abbas N, Markovics A, Muklada H, Ungar ED, Landau SY. 2012. Polyphenols from Pistacia lentiscus and Phillyrea latifolia impair the exsheathment of gastro-intestinal nematode larvae. Veterinary Parasitology, 191, 44–50. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barroso FG, Alados CL, Boza J. 2000. Social hierarchy in the domestic goat: effect on food habits and production. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 69, 35–53. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bell WJ. 1991. Searching behaviour. The behavioral ecology of finding resources. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bernays EA, Singer MS. 2005. Taste alteration and endoparasites. Nature, 436, 476. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bown MD, Poppi DP, Sykes AR. 1991. The effect of post-ruminal infusion of protein or energy on the pathophysiology of Trichostrongylus colubriformis infection and body composition in lambs. Australian Journal of Agricultural Research, 42, 253–267. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown JS. 1988. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 22, 37–47. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burritt EA, Provenza FD. 1991. Ability of lambs to learn with a delay between food ingestion and consequences given meals containing novel and familiar foods. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 32, 179–189. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  11. Caraco T, Blanckenhorn WU, Gregory GM, Newman JA, Recer GM, Zwicker SM. 1990. Risk-sensitivity: ambient temperature affects foraging choice. Animal Behaviour, 39, 338–345. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  12. Catoni C, Peters A, Schaefer M. 2008. Life history trade-offs are influenced by the diversity, availability and interactions of dietary antioxidants. Animal Behaviour, 76, 1107–1119. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  13. Coop RL, Kyriazakis I. 1999. Nutrition-parasite interactions. Veterinary Parasitology, 84, 187–204. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cooper J, Gordon IJ, Pike AW. 2000. Strategies for the avoidance of faeces by grazing sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 69, 15–33. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dunn AM. 1978. Veterinary Helminthology. William Heinemann Medical Books LTD: London. p. 173–185. [Google Scholar]
  16. Forbes AB, Huckle CA, Gibb MJ. 2004. Impact of eprinomectin on grazing behavior and performance in dairy cattle with sub-clinical gastrointestinal nematode infections under continuous stocking management. Veterinary Parasitology, 125, 353–364. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Forbes AB, Huckle CA, Gibb MJ. 2007. Evaluation of the effect of eprinomectin in young dairy heifers sub-clinically infected with gastrointestinal nematodes on grazing behaviour and diet selection. Veterinary Parasitology, 150, 321–332. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fox NJ, Marion G, Davidson RS, White PCL, Hutchings MR. 2013. Modelling parasite transmission in a grazing system: the importance of host behaviour and immunity. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e77996. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ferreira AM, Araujo SS, Sales-Baptista E, Almeida AM. 2012. Identification of novel genes for bitter taste receptors in sheep (Ovis aries). Animal, 7, 547–554. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Galef BG, Jr. 1991. Social factors in diet selection and poison avoidance by Norway Rats: A brief review, in Appetite and Nutrition. Friedman MI, Tordoff MG, Kare MR, Editors. Marcel Deckker: New York. p. 177–194. [Google Scholar]
  21. Garcia J, Hankins WG. 1975. The evolution of bitter and the acquisition of toxiphobia, in Olfaction and Taste, in V. Proceedings of the 5th International Symposium in Melbourne Australia. Denton DA, Coghlan JP, Editors. Academic Press: New York. p. 39–45. [Google Scholar]
  22. Garcia-Gonzalez R, Cuartas P. 1989. A comparison of the diets of the wild goat (Capra pyrenaica), domestic goat (Capra hircus), mouflon (Ovis musimon) and domestic sheep (Ovis aries) in the Cazorla mountain range. Acta Biologica Montana, 9, 123–132. [Google Scholar]
  23. Gardiner MR. 1966. Pathological Changes and Vitamin B12 Metabolism in Sheep Parasitised by Haemonchus contortus, Ostertagia spp. and Trichostrongylus colubriformis. Journal of Helminthology, 40, 63–76. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Glasser TA, Ungar ED, Landau SY, Perevolotsky A, Muklada H, Walker JW. 2009. Breed and maternal effects on the intake of tannin-rich browse by juvenile domestic goats (Capra hircus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 119, 71–77. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  25. Glasser TA, Landau SY, Ungar ED, Perevolotsky A, Dvash L, Muklada H, Kababya D, Walker JW. 2012. Foraging selectivity of three goat breeds in a Mediterranean shrubland. Small Ruminant Research, 102, 7–12. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  26. Glendinning JI. 1992. Effect of salivary proline-rich proteins on ingestive responses to tannic acid in mice. Chemical Senses, 17, 1–12. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  27. Glendinning JI. 1994. Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive? Physiology & Behavior, 56, 1217–1227. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Glendinning JI. 2007. How do predators cope with chemically defended foods? Biological Bulletin, 213, 252–266. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  29. Glendinning JI, Yiin YM, Ackroff K, Sclafani A. 2008. Intragastric infusion of denatonium conditions flavor aversions and delays gastric emptying in rodents. Physiology & Behavior, 93, 757–765. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gradé JT, Tabuti JRS, Van Damme P. 2009. Four footed pharmacists: indications of self-medicating livestock in Karamoja, Uganda. Economic Botany, 63, 29–42. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hart BL. 1988. Biological basis of the behavior of sick animals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 12, 123–137. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hart BL. 2005. The evolution of herbal medicine: behavioural perspectives. Animal Behaviour, 70, 975–989. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  33. Huffman MA. 1997. Current evidence for self-medication in primates: a multidisciplinary perspective. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40, 171–200. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  34. Huffman MA, Ohigashi H, Kawanaka M, Page JE, Kirby GC, Gasquet M, Murakami A, Koshimizu K. 1998. African great ape self-medication: A new paradigm for treating parasite disease with natural medicines? in Towards Natural Medicine Research in the 21st Century. Ageta H, Aimi N, Ebizuka Y, Fujita T, Honda G, Editors. Elsevier Science B.V: Amsterdam. p. 113–123. [Google Scholar]
  35. Huffman MA, Seifu M. 1989. Observations on the illness and consumption of a possibly medicinal plant Vernonia amygdalina by a wild chimpanzee in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 30, 51–63. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  36. Huffman MA. 2001. Self-medicative behavior in the African great apes: An evolutionary perspective into the origins of human traditional medicine. BioScience, 51, 651–661. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  37. Huffman MA. 2007. Primate self-medication, in Primates in Perspective. Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Panger M, Bearder SK, Editors. Oxford University Press: Oxford. p. 677–690. [Google Scholar]
  38. Huffman MA, Spiezio C, Sgaravatti A, Leca JB. 2010. Leaf swallowing behavior in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Biased learning and the emergence of group level cultural differences. Animal Cognition, 13, 871–880. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hutchings MR, Gordon IJ, Kyriazakis I, Robertson E, Jackson F. 2002. Grazing in heterogeneous environments: infra- and supra- parasite distributions determine herbivore grazing decisions. Oecolagia, 132, 453–460. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hutchings MR, Athanasiadou S, Kyriazakis I, Gordon I. 2003. Can animals use foraging behaviour to combat parasites? Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 62, 361–370. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jackson F, Miller J. 2006. Alternative approaches to control-quo vadit? Veterinary Parasitology, 139, 371–384. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Jackson F, Varady M, Bartley DJ. 2012. Managing anthelmintic resistance in goats – Can we learn lessons from sheep? Small Ruminant Research, 103, 3–9. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  43. Janzen J. 1978. Complications in interpreting the chemical defenses of trees against tropical arboreal plant-eating vertebrates, in The Ecology of Arboreal Folivores. Montgomery G, Editor. Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington, D.C., USA. p. 73–84. [Google Scholar]
  44. Juhnke J, Miller J, Hall JO, Provenza FD, Villalba JJ. 2012. Preference for condensed tannins by sheep in response to challenge infection with Haemonchus contortus. Veterinary Parasitology, 188, 104–114. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kababya D, Perevolotsky A, Bruckental I, Landau S. 1998. Selection of diets by dual-purpose Mamber goats in Mediterranean woodland. Journal of Agricultural Science (Cambridge), 131, 221–228. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kalat JW. 1974. Taste salience depends on novelty, not concentration, in taste-aversion learning in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 47–50. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kidane A, Houdijk J, Athanasiadou S, Tolkamp B, Kyriazakis I. 2013. Nutritional sensitivity of periparturient resistance to nematode parasites in two breeds of sheep with different nutrient demands. British Journal of Nutrition, 104, 1477–1486. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  48. Knox MR, Torres-Acosta JFJ, Aguilar-Caballero AJ. 2006. Exploiting the effect of dietary supplementation of small ruminants on resilience and resistance against gastrointestinal nematodes. Veterinary Parasitology, 139, 385–393. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Koshimizu K, Ohigashi H, Huffman M. 1994. Use of Vernonia amygdalina by wild chimpanzee: Possible roles of its bitter and related constituents. Physiology & Behavior, 56, 1209–1216. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kyriazakis I, Tolkamp BJ, Hutchings MR. 1998. Towards a functional explanation for the occurrence of anorexia during parasitic infections. Animal Behaviour, 56, 265–274. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kyriazakis I, Oldham JD, Coop RL, Jackson F. 1994. The effect of subclinical intestinal nematode infection on the diet selection of growing sheep. British Journal of Nutrition, 72, 665–677. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kyriazakis I, Anderson DH, Oldham JD, Coop RL, Jackson F. 1996. Long-term subclinical infection with Trichostrongylus colubriformis: effects on food intake, diet selection and performance of growing lambs. Veterinary Parasitology, 61, 297–313. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kyriazakis I, Tolkamp BJ, Hutchings MR. 1998. Towards a functional explanation for the occurrence of anorexia during parasitic infection. Animal Behaviour, 56, 265–274. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kyriazakis I. 2010. Is anorexia during infection in animals affected by food composition? Animal Feed Science and Technology, 156, 1–9. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lamoot I, Callebaut J, Degezelle T, Demeulenaere E, Laquière J, Vandenberghe C, Hoffmann M. 2004. Eliminative behaviour of free-ranging horses: Do they show latrine behaviour or do they defecate where they graze? Applied Animal Behavior Science, 86, 105–121. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  56. Landau SY, Azaizeh H, Muklada H, Glasser T, Ungar ED, Baram H, Abbas N, Markovics A. 2010. Anthelmintic activity of Pistacia lentiscus foliage in two Middle Eastern breeds of goats differing in their propensity to consume tannin-rich browse. Veterinary Parasitology, 173, 280–286. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Langhans W. 2000. Anorexia of infection: current prospects. Nutrition, 16, 996–1005. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lefèvre T, Oliver L, Hunter MD, De Roode JC. 2010. Evidence for transgenerational medication in nature. Ecology Letters, 13, 1485–1493. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lisonbee LD, Villalba JJ, Provenza FD, Hall JO. 2009. Tannins and self-medication: Implications for sustainable parasite control in herbivores. Behavioural Processes, 82, 184–189. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Martínez-Ortiz de-Montellano C, Vargas-Magaña JJ, Canul-Ku HL, Miranda-Soberanis R, Capetillo-Leal C, Sandoval-Castro CA, Hoste H, Torres-Acosta JFJ. 2010. Effect of a tropical tannin-rich plant Lysiloma latisiliquum on adult populations of Haemonchus contortus in sheep. Veterinary Parasitology, 172, 283–290. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Min BR, Hart SP. 2003. Tannins for suppression of internal parasites. Journal of Animal Science, 81, E102–E109. [Google Scholar]
  62. Mirza SN, Provenza FD. 1990. Preference of the mother affects selection and avoidance of foods by lambs differing in age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 28, 255–263. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mirza SN, Provenza FD. 1992. Effects of age and conditions of exposure on maternally mediated food selection by lambs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 33, 35–42. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mueller-Harvey I. 2006. Unravelling the conundrum of tannins in animal nutrition and health. Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture, 86, 2010–2037. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  65. Osoro K, Mateos-Sanz A, Frutos P, Garcia U, Ortega-Mora LM, Ferreira LMM, Celaya R, Ferre I. 2007. Anthelmintic and nutritional effects of heather supplementation on Cashmere goats grazing perennial ryegrass-white clover pastures. Journal of Animal Science, 85, 861–870. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Palo RT, Robbins CT. 1991. Plant defenses against mammalian herbivory. CRC Press: Boca Raton. [Google Scholar]
  67. Pelchat ML, Grill HJ, Rozin P, Jacobs J. 1983. Quality of acquired responses to tastes by Rattus norvegicus depends on type of associated discomfort. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 97, 140–153. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Poppi DP, Sykes AR, Dynes RA. 1990. The effect of endoparasitism on host nutrition: the implications for nutrient manipulation. Proceedings of the New Zealand Society of Animal Production, 50, 237–243. [Google Scholar]
  69. Provenza FD. 1995. Postingestive feedback as an elementary determinant of food preference and intake in ruminants. Journal of Range Management, 48, 2–17. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  70. Provenza FD, Villalba JJ, Cheney CD, Werner SJ. 1998. Self-organization of foraging behavior: from simplicity to complexity without goals. Nutrition Research Reviews, 11, 199–222. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Provenza FD, Villalba JJ. 2006. Foraging in domestic herbivores: Linking the Internal and External Milieu, in Feeding in Domestic Vertebrates: From Structure to Function. Bels VL, Editor. CABI Publ: Oxfordshire. p. 210–240. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sanga U, Provenza FD, Villalba JJ. 2011. Transmission of self-medicative behaviour from mother to offspring in sheep. Animal Behaviour, 82, 219–227. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  73. Shrader AM, Kerley GIH, Kotler BP, Brown JS. 2007. Social information, social feeding, and competition in group-living goats (Capra hircus). Behavioral Ecology, 18, 103–107. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  74. Singer MS, Mace KC, Bernays EA. 2009. Self-medication as adaptive plasticity: increased ingestion of plant toxins by parasitized caterpillars. PLoS ONE, 4(3), e4796. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sykes AR. 1983. Effects of parasitism on metabolism in the sheep, in Sheep Production. Haresign W, Editor. Butterworths: London. p. 317–334. [Google Scholar]
  76. Sykes AR. 1987. Endoparasites and herbivore nutrition, in The Nutrition of Herbivores. Hacker JB, Ternouth JH, Editors. Academic Press: Sydney. p. 211–232. [Google Scholar]
  77. Symons LEA. 1985. Anorexia: occurrence, pathophysiology and possible causes in parasitic infections. Advances in Parasitology, 24, 103–133. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Szyszka O, Tolkamp BJ, Edwards SA, Kyriazakis I. 2013. Do the changes in the behaviours of cattle during parasitism with Ostertagia ostertagi have a potential diagnostic value? Veterinary Parasitology, 193, 214–222. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Thorhallsdottir AG, Provenza FD, Balph DF. 1990. Ability of lambs to learn about novel foods while observing or participating with social models. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 25, 25–33. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  80. Torregosa AM, Dearing MD. 2009. Nutritional toxicology of mammals: regulated intake of plant secondary compounds. Functional Ecology, 23, 48–56. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  81. Vargas-Magaña JJ, Aguilar-Caballero AJ, Torres-Acosta JF, Sandoval-Castro CA, Hoste H, Capetillo-Leal CM. 2013. Tropical tannin-rich fodder intake modifies saliva-binding capacity in growing sheep. Animal, 7, 1921–1924. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Villalba JJ, Provenza FD. 1996. Preference for flavored wheat straw by lambs conditioned with intraruminal administrations of sodium propionate. Journal of Animal Science, 74, 2362–2368. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Villalba JJ, Provenza FD. 1997a. Preference for wheat straw by lambs conditioned with intraruminal infusions of starch. British Journal of Nutrition, 77, 287–297. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  84. Villalba JJ, Provenza FD. 1997b. Preference for flavored foods by lambs conditioned with intraruminal administrations of nitrogen. British Journal of Nutrition, 78, 545–561. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  85. Villalba JJ, Provenza FD. 1997c. Preference for flavored wheat straw by lambs conditioned with intraruminal infusions of acetate and propionate. Journal of Animal Science, 75, 2905–2914. [Google Scholar]
  86. Villalba JJ, Provenza FD, Rogosic J. 1999. Preference for flavored wheat straw by lambs conditioned with intraruminal infusions of starch administered at different times after straw ingestion. Journal of Animal Science, 77, 3185–3190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Villalba JJ, Provenza FD, Hall JO, Lisonbee LD. 2010. Selection of tannins by sheep in response to gastrointestinal nematode infection. Journal of Animal Science, 88, 2189–2198. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Villalba JJ, Landau SY. 2012. Host behaviour, environment and ability to self-medicate. Small Ruminant Research, 103, 50–59. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  89. Villalba JJ, Miller J, Hall JO, Clemensen AK, Stott R, Snyder D, Provenza FD. 2013. Preference for tanniferous (Onobrychis viciifolia) and non-tanniferous (Astragalus cicer) forage plants by sheep in response to challenge infection with Haemonchus contortus. Small Ruminant Research, 112, 199–207. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  90. Vitazkova SK, Long E, Paul A, Glendinning JI. 2001. Mice suppress malaria infection by sampling a “bitter” chemotherapy agent. Animal Behaviour, 61, 887–894. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  91. Zahorik DM, Houpt KA, Swartzman-Andert J. 1990. Taste-aversion learning in three species of ruminants. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 26, 27–39. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  92. Zhu X, He L, McCluskey LP. 2014. Ingestion of bacterial lipopolysaccharide inhibits peripheral taste responses to sucrose in mice. Neuroscience, 258, 47–61. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Cite this article as: Villalba JJ, Miller J, Ungar ED, Landau SY & Glendinning J: Ruminant self-medication against gastrointestinal nematodes: evidence, mechanism, and originsII. Parasite, 2014, 21, 31.

All Figures

thumbnail Figure 1.

(A) Gastrointestinal parasites cause pain and discomfort. Non-medicinal foods associated with such sensations may lead to an acquired distaste. (B) In contrast, medicinal antiparasitic foods that promote relief can lead to a preference, i.e., self-medication.

In the text
thumbnail Figure 2.

A conceptual representation of how pre- and post-ingestive events control the manifestation of self-medicative behavior in mammalian herbivores. Self-medication emerges from enhanced neophilia and increased acceptance of certain somatosensations (e.g., taste dimensions, tactile properties) triggered by parasitism. These increases in neophilia and acceptability, together with social learning, should “prime” animals to ingest therapeutic doses of medicinal secondary compounds (pre-ingestive processes). Subsequently, associative learning (i.e., associations between orosensorial properties of a medicinal food and relief experienced after ingesting that food) will maintain and/or reinforce self-medicative behaviors. Thus, a chain of events starting with food acceptability and social learning followed by post-ingestive processes may contribute to the emergence of self-medication in mammalian herbivores.

In the text

Current usage metrics show cumulative count of Article Views (full-text article views including HTML views, PDF and ePub downloads, according to the available data) and Abstracts Views on Vision4Press platform.

Data correspond to usage on the plateform after 2015. The current usage metrics is available 48-96 hours after online publication and is updated daily on week days.

Initial download of the metrics may take a while.